Personality Judgments in Dogs and Humans
Order ID# 45178248544XXTG457 Plagiarism Level: 0-0.5% Writer Classification: PhD competent Style: APA/MLA/Harvard/Chicago Delivery: Minimum 3 Hours Revision: Permitted Sources: 4-6 Course Level: Masters/University College Guarantee Status: 96-99% Instructions
Personality Judgments in Dogs and Humans
A Dog’s Got Personality: A Cross-Species Comparative Approach to
Personality Judgments in Dogs and Humans
Samuel D. Gosling
University of Texas at Austin
Virginia S. Y. Kwan
Princeton University
Oliver P. John
University of California, Berkeley
This research offers a blueprint for how a cross-species comparative approach can be realized empirically. In a single design, parallel procedures and instruments were used in 2 species, dogs (Canis
familiaris) and humans (Homo sapiens), to test whether personality differences exist and can be judged
in dogs as accurately as in humans. Personality judgments of humans and dogs were compared on 3
accuracy criteria: internal consistency, consensus, and correspondence. Results showed that, on all 3
criteria, judgments of dogs were as accurate as judgments of humans. These findings are consistent with
the evolutionary continuity hypothesis and suggest an important conclusion not widely considered by
either personality or animal researchers: Personality differences do exist and can be measured in animals
other than humans.
“A dog’s got personality and personality goes a long way.” So
said Jules Winnfield, a gangster from the movie Pulp Fiction.
Fictional gangsters are not the only ones concerned with personality in nonhuman animals. Robert Fagen, a professor of Biometry,
used the personality traits “irascible, irritable, manipulative, and
grumpy” to describe Suzy June, a brown bear he had observed for
several years (Aschenbach, 1995). Although scientists such as
Fagen are beginning to apply personality constructs to animals, a
systematic empirical evaluation of such personality judgments has
yet to be performed. In this report, we focus on personality
judgments of domestic dogs, directly comparing them with personality judgments of humans.
Animal models have played a central role in much psychological science (Domjan & Purdy, 1995). Yet, although scientists
widely accept the idea that the anatomy and physiology of humans
show considerable continuity to other mammals, most have been
reluctant to ascribe emotions and personality traits to animals. As
a result, there are few systematic studies on animals and crossdisciplinary bridges between personality psychologists and
animal-behavior researchers are virtually nonexistent (Gosling,
2001). Most questions about animal personality remain untested,
and a field of animal personality remains to be developed.
However, there is nothing in evolutionary theory to suggest that
only physical traits are subject to selection pressures. Indeed,
Darwin (1872/1998) argued explicitly that emotions exist in both
human and nonhuman animals, including primates, cats, and dogs.
Similarly, personality traits like Neuroticism, Extraversion, and
Agreeableness may not be as uniquely human as once was thought
(Buss, 1988; Gosling & John, 1999; Hogan, 1996). In this article,
we examine a core issue for animal-personality research: whether
differences in personality traits exist and can be measured in
animals.
To show that personality traits exist in nonhuman animals,
assessments of animal personality must be proven accurate. A
small number of studies have reported reliabilities, but systematic
efforts to examine the accuracy of assessment methods have been
rare (see Gosling, 2001, for a review of the studies available). This
dearth of studies may reflect the challenges of assessing creatures
that cannot speak to us and may not have the mental or physical
faculties (e.g., conceptions of self, autobiographical memory) required by the tests of personality developed for humans. However,
there is one method that may be suited for use in animals—
judgments made by informants who are well acquainted with the
target individuals. It should be noted that this personality-judgment
approach (Funder, 1999) is far from a method of last resort. As
Hofstee (1994) noted about personality in humans, “The averaged
judgment of knowledgeable others provides the best available
point of reference for both the definition of personality structure in
general and for assessing someone’s personality in particular” (p.
149). Thus, many human-personality researchers consider judgSamuel D. Gosling, Department of Psychology, University of Texas at
Austin; Virginia S. Y. Kwan, Department of Psychology, Princeton University; Oliver P. John, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley.
Preparation of this article was supported by a research grant from the
University of Texas College of Liberal Arts to Samuel D. Gosling and by
National Institutes of Mental Health Grants MH43948 and MH49255 for
the work of Oliver P. John and Virginia S. Y. Kwan. We are grateful to
Asenath Bartley, Allison Bonburg, and Angela Huddleston for their help in
collecting some of the data reported here, and to Dan Ozer for his
thoughtful comments on an earlier version of this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Samuel
- Gosling, Department of Psychology, 1 University Station A8000,
University of Texas, Austin, Texas 78712-0187. E-mail: gosling@
psy.utexas.edu
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
2003, Vol. 85, No. 6, 1161–1169 0022-3514/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1161
1161
ments by knowledgeable informants or observers the sine qua non
of personality (Funder, 1995; Kenny, 1994; McCrae, 1982; Wiggins, 1973), making such judgments a logical candidate for studies
of animals. We thus propose to extend the personality-judgment
approach to research on animals; specifically, we test whether
personality traits exist and can be judged in one particular nonhuman species, dogs.
Introducing a Cross-Species Comparative Approach to
Personality
How should such research be performed? The logic of comparative research suggests that multiple species should be examined,
preferably using at least one well-studied species. Such crossspecies designs are useful because they provide researchers with a
benchmark against which to evaluate results for a newly examined
species. Given that we know more about the personality of humans
than of any other species, humans should be one of the species
included.
What should the other species be? Animal-personality research
is most likely to be conducted in research facilities, zoos, farms,
and animal shelters, on species such as rhesus monkeys, chimpanzees, sheep, and dogs. We focus on domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) because (a) they are widely owned, making them a readily
available source of subjects, (b) they can safely and naturally
engage in wide array of behaviors, (c) their behavioral repertoire is
well understood by a large pool of observers, and (d) they can
travel safely to research sites without specialist care. Together,
these reasons make them a manageable species for research purposes (cf. lions, elephants), especially in the nascent stages of
research on animal personality.
Domestic dogs are found throughout the world, living alongside
humans, an association traced as far back as 14,000 to 20,000 years
ago. The evolutionary origins of domestic dogs are still subject to
debate (Nowak, 1999). Some authorities have suggested that modern dogs were derived from one or a few wolf subspecies and then
spread throughout the world in association with people (Nowak,
1979). Others have suggested that domestication occurred at different times and different locations with humans domesticating
whichever local species of canid happened to be around (Coren,
1994). In any event, many of the modern morphological and
behavioral characteristics associated with modern domestic dogs
have been selected within the context of human–dog relationships
since domestication (Hare, Brown, Williamson, & Tomasello,
2002).
Criteria for Evaluating Accuracy
If we are going to use human judgments as data for animalpersonality research, these judgments need to be subjected to the
same rigorous accuracy evaluations as judgments of human personality (Funder, 1995). Although the specific criteria and how
they are labeled vary somewhat from theorist to theorist, human
personality judgments have been evaluated with respect to three
major accuracy criteria: internal consistency, consensus, and correspondence (e.g., Kruglanski, 1989; Robins & John, 1997). Internal consistency reflects the degree to which judgments about an
individual’s personality are consistent across observations or items
thought to reflect the same behavioral dimension (Robins & John,
1997; Wiggins, 1973). Consensus implies agreement among independent informants or observers, and is often considered the hallmark of accuracy (e.g., Block, 1961; Funder, 1995; Kenny, 1994;
Kruglanski, 1989; McCrae, 1982). Correspondence refers to the
extent to which judgments predict an external criterion for “reality” (Kruglanski, 1989); perhaps the most valuable external criterion is independent observations of behaviors (Funder, 1995;
Kenny, 1994). In the present research, we use these three accuracy
criteria, developed in human research, to evaluate the accuracy of
personality judgments of dogs. For each criterion, we compare the
dog findings with parallel findings from humans. If personality
traits do not exist in dogs, then judgments of dogs should fall short
on these criteria when compared with judgments of humans.
The most knowledgeable informants about dogs are typically
their owners, who have extensively observed the animals in their
care, across both situations and time. Therefore, we focused our
judgment approach on how the personality of dogs is judged by
their owners. To estimate consensus between the owner and an
independent judge, we obtained judgments of the dogs by a second
person familiar with the target animals (a “peer”). Internal consistency was studied in the judgments of both informants (owners
and peers). Finally, we tested the correspondence of the owner’s
judgments by comparing them with the dog’s behavior rated by
independent observers in a local dog park.
Judgments of dogs, just like judgments of people, may be based
on physical and appearance characteristics, rather than actual behavior. Thus, we also took photographs of the dogs, permitting us
to obtain personality assessments on the basis of appearance alone;
these data allowed us to test the extent to which assessments of
dogs are affected by breed and other appearance stereotypes. In
addition, we controlled for such other background characteristics
as sex and age that can influence personality judgments.
To implement a cross-species comparative approach, we obtained personality judgments not only of the dogs but also of their
human owners. In this cross-species design, we used parallel
procedures, instruments, and constructs for both dogs and humans,
allowing us to compare dog findings directly with human findings
within the same study.
Lessons Learned From Previous Research
Studies of animal personality are isolated and few and far
between. Reports are scattered across a multitude of disciplines
and journals, ranging from veterinary medicine and zoology to
agricultural science and psychology (e.g., Capitanio, 1999; Fairbanks, 2001; King & Figueredo, 1997; Sinn, Perrin, Mather, &
Anderson, 2001). With few systematic attempts to assess personality in dogs and other nonhuman species, comprehensive evaluations of the accuracy of personality judgments have not been
possible. Nonetheless, a number of useful lessons can be learned
by surveying the limitations of the few studies that do exist. These
lessons have guided the design of the present research.
Previous studies of personality in dogs have not been comprehensive in their coverage of relevant traits (e.g., Murphy, 1995)
and behavioral domains (e.g., Cattell & Korth, 1973), even though
the importance of examining a broad array of trait dimensions is
now widely understood in the human literature (John & Srivastava,
1999). Another important reason for examining multiple traits in
the same study is to address discriminant validity. Are personality
1162 GOSLING, KWAN, AND JOHN
ratings on multiple dimensions independent and valid, or do they
simply reflect a single evaluative dimension, such as “like–
dislike”? To address this possibility, human research on personality judgments has assessed multiple traits simultaneously, permitting an evaluation of each dimension while controlling for
judgments of the other dimensions. The same now needs to be
done with personality dimensions in dogs.
In some cases, previous studies of dogs have assessed personality dimensions using only one item (e.g., Murphy, 1995; Slabbert
& Odendaal, 1999). Single items are problematic because they
provide less reliable measurements than do multiple indicators and
do not permit the assessment of internal consistency (Gosling,
Rentfrow, & Swann, in press). Thus, personality dimensions
should be estimated using multiple items.
Moreover, research on dogs has focused on only one accuracy
criterion at a time, rather than systematically testing all the important aspects of accuracy. For example, there may be internal
consistency, or there may be consensus, but if judgments reflect
only shared stereotypes about breed, then there may be little
correspondence. Similarly, if consensus is low, correspondence
might be severely underestimated. Thus, all three accuracy criteria
need to be tested in the same study.
Most previous studies of dogs have been done in applied contexts where the behavior of dogs is of interest in and of itself (e.g.,
Slabbert & Odendaal, 1999; Svartberg, 2002). By focusing on one
species, rather than taking the comparative approach proposed
here, these studies make it difficult to evaluate the broader significance of the findings. Should a consensus correlation of .40 be
considered large or small? Some kind of benchmark is needed
against which the findings from dogs can be evaluated. The accuracy of personality judgments has been studied most extensively in
humans, making them the most appropriate benchmark species.
One limitation of some of the previous research is that it has
examined dog breeds globally, rather than judgments of individual
dogs (e.g., Coren, 1998; Hart & Miller, 1985). These studies have
shown good consensus among experts in judging breed-typical
characteristics; however, they do not address the accuracy of
judgments of individual dogs. To illustrate this point, consider that
judges might well agree about the stereotypical traits associated
with the peoples of England, Hong Kong, and Germany, yet
relying on these national stereotypes may be of little value for
judging the personalities of particular individuals. Indeed, such
national stereotypes may even lead judges to overlook the individuating behaviors of the individuals. Nonetheless, the previous
work showing personality differences among dog breeds is important for interpreting findings on differences among individual
dogs; researchers must ensure that personality judgments indeed
reflect real behaviors, rather than impressions based on breed
differences or other stereotypes and biases (e.g., based on sex, age,
size, or appearance).
In conclusion, to achieve a more complete understanding and
evaluation of dog-personality judgments, research should: (a) include a comprehensive set of trait dimensions, (b) measure each
dimension with multiple items, (c) evaluate multiple accuracy
criteria, (d) assess two or more species in a comparative design,
and (e) assess judgments of individuals (rather than groups or
breeds) while controlling for the potentially biasing effects of
stereotypes, such as those related to sex, age, and physical appearance. This kind of comprehensive species-comparative design has
not yet been implemented but is urgently needed if the field of
animal personality is to emerge from its infancy.
Overview of Studies
Dog owners and their dogs were recruited in a local dog park to
participate in three studies. In Study 1, each owner provided
personality judgments of their dog as well as of their own personality. The owners also identified another person (a “peer”) who
was familiar with both the target dog and the owner and could thus
judge both their personalities. This cross-species design allowed us
to compare (a) the internal consistency of dog and human personality judgments and (b) the consensus between owner and peer in
judging either dog or human personality. In Study 2, owners
brought their dogs to a field-testing enclosure located at a dog
park, where the dogs’ behaviors were observed and rated by three
independent observers in an observational field-testing session. To
assess the correspondence criterion of accuracy, we tested how
well the owners’ personality judgments of their dogs predicted the
behavior ratings obtained in the field-testing sessions. In Study 3,
photographs of the dogs (taken at the dog park) were rated by a
new set of observers. These photo-based ratings allowed us to
examine the effects of breed and appearance characteristics.
Personality Judgments by Owner and Peer Informants:
The Five-Factor Model (FFM) as a Framework
Which personality dimensions should be assessed? Although
several factor-analytic studies have identified dimensions of dog
personality (e.g., Cattell & Korth, 1973; Svartberg & Forkman,
2002), no single model has been adopted by the field. Using the
FFM as an organizing framework, Gosling and John (1999) summarized the structural findings from the factor analytic studies of
dogs and 11 other species. One of the most striking findings to
emerge was that Conscientiousness did not appear as an independent personality dimension in dogs; in fact, Conscientiousness
appeared only in humans and humans’ closest relatives, namely
chimpanzees (King & Figueredo, 1997). This pattern of findings
suggests that Conscientiousness appeared as a distinct dimension
of personality relatively recently in evolutionary history, long after
canid ancestors diverged from hominid ancestors (Gosling & John,
1999).
These review findings, along with the results of a series of
comprehensive trait studies (Gosling & John, 1998), suggest that at
this point, a four-dimensional model is the most promising for
personality traits in dogs. These four dimensions represent canine
analogs of four of the five human FFM factors: Energy (analogous
to human Extraversion), Affection (analogous to human Agreeableness), Emotional Reactivity (analogous to human Neuroticism), and Intelligence (analogous to human Openness/Intellect).
Thus, we focused on these four dimensions, which currently provide the most reasonable, albeit provisional, model for organizing
personality judgments of dogs. With no evidence for a separate
Conscientiousness dimension in any species other than humans
and chimpanzees, it would make little sense to assess this trait in
dogs.
RUBRIC
Excellent Quality 95-100%
Introduction 45-41 points
The background and significance of the problem and a clear statement of the research purpose is provided. The search history is mentioned.
Literature Support 91-84 points
The background and significance of the problem and a clear statement of the research purpose is provided. The search history is mentioned.
Methodology 58-53 points
Content is well-organized with headings for each slide and bulleted lists to group related material as needed. Use of font, color, graphics, effects, etc. to enhance readability and presentation content is excellent. Length requirements of 10 slides/pages or less is met.
Average Score 50-85%
40-38 points More depth/detail for the background and significance is needed, or the research detail is not clear. No search history information is provided.
83-76 points Review of relevant theoretical literature is evident, but there is little integration of studies into concepts related to problem. Review is partially focused and organized. Supporting and opposing research are included. Summary of information presented is included. Conclusion may not contain a biblical integration.
52-49 points Content is somewhat organized, but no structure is apparent. The use of font, color, graphics, effects, etc. is occasionally detracting to the presentation content. Length requirements may not be met.
Poor Quality 0-45%
37-1 points The background and/or significance are missing. No search history information is provided.
75-1 points Review of relevant theoretical literature is evident, but there is no integration of studies into concepts related to problem. Review is partially focused and organized. Supporting and opposing research are not included in the summary of information presented. Conclusion does not contain a biblical integration.
48-1 points There is no clear or logical organizational structure. No logical sequence is apparent. The use of font, color, graphics, effects etc. is often detracting to the presentation content. Length requirements may not be met
You Can Also Place the Order at www.perfectacademic.com/orders/ordernow or www.crucialessay.com/orders/ordernow